Difference: C-TOCLiteratureReview (85 vs. 86)

Revision 862011-06-06 - MatthewBrehmer

Line: 1 to 1
 
META TOPICPARENT name="C-TOC"

Literature Review Notes

Line: 6 to 6
 
Added:
>
>

Methodology

  • Kaptein, M. C., Nass, C., & Markopoulos, P. (2010). Powerful and consistent analysis of likert-type ratingscales. CHI ’10 (pp. 2391-2394). New York, New York, USA: ACM Press. doi: 10.1145/1753326.1753686.

  • Wobbrock, J. O., Findlater, L., Gergle, D., & Higgins, J. J. (2011). The Aligned Rank Transform for Nonparametric Factorial Analyses Using Only A NOVA Procedures. CHI 2011 (pp. 143-146).
 

HCI & Universal Usability

On universal usability (general)

Line: 1298 to 1305
 
  • how to include reminders of main task / ensure deep rehearsal in UI design? which environmental cues to use? recency, frequency
  • social obligations of interruptions
Added:
>
>

[Oulasvirta 06]

Oulasvirta, A., & Saariluoma, P. (2006). Surviving task interruptions: Investigating the implications of long-term working memory theory. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 64(10), 941-961. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.04.006.

  • interruptions have no effect on memory when LTWM encoding speed is fast enough for task processing, regardless of pacing, intensity, difficulty;
  • cost of interruptions = memory loss - omissions and errors;
  • safeguarding task representations;
  • individual and task differences in interruption tolerance;
  • if safeguarding has taken place, resumption of a task should be quick and accurate regardless of processing demands of interrupting task;
  • current theories of STWM, short-term traces are assumed to decay completely, in a time of about 30s, when no rehearsal can be carried out during interruption;
  • theories of working memory based on transient activation of information in LTM cannot explain the resumption of activity once the information in working memory has be irretrievably lost;
  • if an interruption takes place, task representation in LTWM remains in an interrupted but intact state; people can use the few seconds available upon task switching to encode some of those contents to LTWM; when encoding skills do not match the processing demands assumed by the task environment, resumption of the task requires alternative strategies, searching for cues in the environment or generating them based on semantic memory on the task;
  • benefit of interruption lag / opportunity to rehearse disappeared with task practice;
  • processing demands of interruption task does not affect memory for main task (in contrast to [Gillie 89]); rehearsal used as maintenance strategy; retrieval structures not used for meaningless stimuli;
  • no evidence for transient activation, refreshing activation during interruption lag / interruption
  • length of interruption can matter i conditions of massive interference and where rehearsal is rules our by experimental manipulation;
  • memory load / task phase are not the factors determining disruptiveness to interruptions although they do coincide with it;
  • extreme time pressure may lead to selection of stimulus-specific processes at the expense of processes relying on a more general representation;
  • semantic access in contrast to surface access;
 

more...

Line: 1354 to 1381
 
  • Oulasvirta, A., & Saariluoma, P. (2004). Long-term working memory and interrupting messages in human - computer interaction. Behaviour & Information Technology, 23(1), 53-64. doi: 10.1080/01449290310001644859.
Deleted:
<
<
  • Oulasvirta, A., & Saariluoma, P. (2006). Surviving task interruptions: Investigating the implications of long-term working memory theory. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 64(10), 941-961. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2006.04.006.
 
  • Oulasvirta, A., & Salovaara, A. (2004). A cognitive meta-analysis of design approaches to interruptions in intelligent environments. Extended abstracts of the 2004 conference on Human factors and computing systems - CHI ’04, 1155. New York, New York, USA: ACM Press. doi: 10.1145/985921.986012.
Added:
>
>
  • Iqbal, S. T., & Bailey, B. P. (2010). Oasis. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 17(4), 1-28. doi: 10.1145/1879831.1879833.

  • Dabbish, L., Mark, G., & González, V. M. (2011). Why do i keep interrupting myself?: environment, habit and self-interruption. CHI 2011 (p. 3127–3130). ACM. Retrieved May 16, 2011, from http://portal.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1979405.
 

On Prospective memory (PM) and interruption/distraction, age-related issues

[Farrimond 06]

Line: 1641 to 1672
 
  • Einstein GO, McDaniel MA, Williford CL, Pagan JL, Dismukes RK. Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. Journal of experimental psychology. Applied. 2003;9(3):147-62. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14570509.
Added:
>
>
  • Uttl, B. (2008). Transparent meta-analysis of prospective memory and aging. PloS one, 3(2), e1568. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001568.
 

On older adults and cognitive ageing, interruptibility and distractibility

[Anderson 00]

Line: 1678 to 1711
 
    • drops in divided but not necessarily sustained attention ; inhibition theory of memory relevant here; unable to suppress distractions and enhance foci; compromised attention influences PS and memory
    • enhancement and suppression [Gazzaley 05] study w/ places and faces; older adults less able to suppress, less able to enhance;
    • PFC responsible for enhancement and suppression, goal activation, attentional modulation - all change w/ age
Deleted:
<
<
    • [West 96] studied PFC change with age and effect on RM, PM, interference control/suppression of distractions, response inhibition (go/no-go), recall and recognition memory; evidence from patients with frontal lobe damage - neural correlates of distraction;
 
    • measures of distractibility: difficult to design studies to measure this; hard to design training programs for mitigating distractions;

more...

Line: 1687 to 1719
 
Changed:
<
<
>
>
  • Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 17(1), 163-9. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1826730.

  • Craik, F. I., Byrd, M., & Swanson, J. M. (1987). Patterns of memory loss in three elderly samples. Psychology and aging, 2(1), 79-86. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3268196.
  • Cabeza, R. (2004). Task-independent and Task-specific Age Effects on Brain Activity during Working Memory, Visual Attention and Episodic Retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 14(4), 364-375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg133.

  • Cabeza, R., Anderson, N., Locantore, J., & Mcintosh, A. (2002). Aging Gracefully: Compensatory Brain Activity in High-Performing Older Adults. NeuroImage, 17(3), 1394-1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280.

  • Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1997). Aging and Mind Wandering: Reduced Inhibition in Older Adults? Experimental Aging Research, 23(4), 343-354. doi: 10.1080/03610739708254035.

  • West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological bulletin, 120(2), 272-92. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8831298.
    • [West 96] studied PFC change with age and effect on RM, PM, interference control/suppression of distractions, response inhibition (go/no-go), recall and recognition memory; evidence from patients with frontal lobe damage - neural correlates of distraction;
 
 
This site is powered by the TWiki collaboration platform Powered by PerlCopyright © 2008-2025 by the contributing authors. All material on this collaboration platform is the property of the contributing authors.
Ideas, requests, problems regarding TWiki? Send feedback